Link Search Menu Expand Document

Do Ethical Attitudes Shape Political Behaviours?

According to Feinberg & Willer (2013, p. 1), ‘moral convictions and the emotions they evoke shape political attitudes.’ What evidence supports this claim? By the end of this unit you should have an initial understanding of how researchers have attempted to gather relevant evidence, and you should be familiar with some evidence for this claim.

If the video isn’t working you could also watch it on youtube. Or you can view just the slides (no audio or video).

This recording is also available on stream (no ads; search enabled).

If the slides are not working, or you prefer them full screen, please try this link. The recording is available on stream and youtube.

Notes

In this section we aim to understand and evaluate the first key claim in the argument that cultural differences in moral psychology matter for political conflict over climate change:

Moral convictions and the emotions they evoke shape political attitudes’ (Feinberg & Willer, 2013, p. 1).

Attitudes Do Not Explain Behaviours

Even strongly held attitudes have little influence on behaviours according to a classic review by Wicker (1969).

‘it is considerably more likely that attitudes will be unrelated or only slightly related to overt behaviors than that attitudes will be closely related to actions’ […] ‘substantial proportions of subjects show attitude-behavior discrepancies. This is true even when subjects scoring at the extremes of attitudinal measures are compared on behavioral indices’ (p. 65).

Genthner & Taylor (1973) on racist prejudice provides a dramatic illustration. Subjects who self-reported greater prejudice were more aggressive overall in applying electric shocks, but ‘aggressed equally against’ both White people and Black people. Racist attitudes and racist behaviours are not always correlated (as many of us may know from experience, unfortunately).

Moral Attitudes Do Explain Behaviours

Skitka, Bauman, & Sargis (2005) contrasted moral attitudes (e.g. about sexuality) with non-moral but extreme attitudes (e.g. about sport). To what extend do people attempt to maintain social distance from others with conflicting attitudes?

‘The effect of moral conviction on social distance was robust when we controlled for the effects gender, age, attitudinal extremity, importance, and centrality’

‘In contrast, participants were more tolerant of having a distant than an intimate relationship with an attitudinally dissimilar other, when the attitude dissimilarity was on an issue that the participant held with low moral conviction, results that held even when we controlled for attitude strength’ (Skitka et al., 2005, p. Study 1).

But Do Moral Attitudes Explain Political Behaviours?

Skitka & Bauman (2008) report that your moral conviction about an election candidate increases both the probability that you will vote (Study 1) and the reported strength of your intention to vote (Study 2).

In both studies: ‘the effects of moral conviction on political engagement were equally strong for those on the political right and left’ (Skitka & Bauman, 2008, p. 50).

We should be cautious in relying on these particular studies insofar as the effects could in principle be due to ‘markers of attitude strength’ other than moral conviction (Skitka & Bauman, 2008, pp. 36–7).

What about Attitudes to Climate Change specifically?

Doran, Böhm, Pfister, Steentjes, & Pidgeon (2019) measured (i) the extent to which subjects took climate change to be a moral concern,1 and (ii) the extent to which subjects evaluated the consequences of climate change negatively.

They found that

‘individuals with strong moral concerns about climate change tend to be more likely to support climate policies.

and that

‘moral concerns [were] substantially more important than consequence evaluations, explaining about twice as much of the variance.’

Conversely, Hornsey, Harris, Bain, & Fielding (2016) contrasted climate sceptics with people who know humans are causing climate change . They found that merely knowing makes little measurable difference to behaviours. (This is discussed in the recording for Moral Psychology Drives Environmental Concern.) As they put it in a later review:

‘knowing whether people are skeptics or believers tells you surprisingly little about their willingness to engage in actions that matter’ (Hornsey & Fielding, 2020, p. 21).

Putting these two findings together (Doran et al., 2019 and Hornsey et al., 2016), knowing about climate change or its consequences does not have much effect on practical support for mitigation compared to perceiving environmental issues as moral issues.

Conclusion

Overall, we appear to have identified some evidence for the claim that ‘Moral convictions and the emotions they evoke shape political attitudes’ (Feinberg & Willer, 2013, p. 1). However, this required us to go beyond the studies those authors themselves cited in support of this claim.

Glossary

moral conviction : ‘Moral conviction refers to a strong and absolute belief that something is right or wrong, moral or immoral’ (Skitka et al., 2005, p. 896).

References

Doran, R., Böhm, G., Pfister, H.-R., Steentjes, K., & Pidgeon, N. (2019). Consequence evaluations and moral concerns about climate change: Insights from nationally representative surveys across four European countries. Journal of Risk Research, 22(5), 610–626. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2018.1473468
Feinberg, M., & Willer, R. (2013). The Moral Roots of Environmental Attitudes. Psychological Science, 24(1), 56–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612449177
Genthner, R., & Taylor, S. (1973). Physical aggression as a function of racial prejudice and the race of the target. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27(2), 207–210.
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015141
Hornsey, M. J., & Fielding, K. S. (2020). Understanding (and Reducing) Inaction on Climate Change. Social Issues and Policy Review, 14(1), 3–35. https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12058
Hornsey, M. J., Harris, E. A., Bain, P. G., & Fielding, K. S. (2016). Meta-analyses of the determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change. Nature Climate Change, 6(6), 622–626. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2943
Skitka, L. J. (2010). The Psychology of Moral Conviction. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(4), 267–281. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00254.x
Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C., & Sargis, E. (2005). Moral Conviction: Another Contributor to Attitude Strength or Something More? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(6), 895–917.
Skitka, L. J., & Bauman, C. W. (2008). Moral Conviction and Political Engagement. Political Psychology, 29(1), 29–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2007.00611.x
Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus Actions: The Relationship of Verbal and Overt Behavioral Responses to Attitude Objects. Journal of Social Issues, 25(4), 41–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1969.tb00619.x
  1. In this research, the question about moral concern was:

    ‘Some people have moral concerns about climate change. For example, because they think that its harmful impacts are more likely to affect poorer countries, or because they feel a moral responsibility towards future generations’ (Doran et al., 2019, p. 615)

    This appears to highlight the harm and fairness rather than any of the binding moral foundations such as purity. If Graham, Haidt, & Nosek (2009) are right about cultural differences and political orientation, this might in principle mean that the study confounded moral concern with political orientation.